Imagerie et cognition
Free Access
Issue
Med Sci (Paris)
Volume 27, Number 1, Janvier 2011
Imagerie et cognition
Page(s) 77 - 81
Section M/S revues
DOI https://doi.org/10.1051/medsci/201127177
Published online 10 February 2011
  1. Laureys S. The neural correlate of (un)awareness: lessons from the vegetative state. Trends Cogn Sci 2005 ; 9 : 556-559. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bernat JL. Chronic consciousness disorders. Annu Rev Med 2009 ; 60 : 381-392. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Demertzi, A, Laureys S, Boly M. Coma, persistent vegetative states, and diminished consciousness. In : Banks WP, ed. Encyclopedia of consciousness. Oxford : Elsevier, 2009 : 147-156. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  4. Medical aspects of the persistent vegetative state (1). The multi-society task force on PVS. N Engl J Med 1994 ; 330 : 1499-1508. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Jennett B, Plum F. Persistent vegetative state after brain damage. A syndrome in search of a name. Lancet 1972 ; 1 : 734-737. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. American congress of rehabilitation medicine. Recommendations for use of uniform nomenclature pertinent to patients with severe alterations in consciousness. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 1995 ; 76 : 205-209. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Giacino JT, Ashwal S, Childs N, et al. The minimally conscious state: definition and diagnostic criteria. Neurology 2002 ; 58 : 349-353. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Plum F, Posner JB. The diagnosis of stupor and coma. Philadelphie : Davis FA, 1966. [Google Scholar]
  9. Schnakers C, Majerus S, Goldman S, et al. Cognitive function in the locked-in syndrome. J Neurol 2008 ; 255 : 323-330. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Schnakers C, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Giacino J, et al. Diagnostic accuracy of the vegetative and minimally conscious state: clinical consensus versus standardized neurobehavioral assessment. BMC Neurol 2009 ; 9 : 35. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bruno MA, Schnakers C, Damas F, et al. Locked-in syndrome in children: report of five cases and review of the literature. Pediatr Neurol 2009 ; 41 : 237-246. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Gill-Thwaites H. Lotteries, loopholes and luck: misdiagnosis in the vegetative state patient. Brain Inj 2006 ; 20 : 1321-1328. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Vanhaudenhuyse A, Schnakers C, Bredart S, Laureys S. Assessment of visual pursuit in post-comatose states: use a mirror. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008 ; 79 : 223. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. DeVolder AG, Goffinet AM, Bol A, et al. Brain glucose metabolism in postanoxic syndrome. Positron emission tomographic study. Arch Neurol 1990 ; 47 : 197-204. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Laureys S, Goldman S, Phillips C, et al. Impaired effective cortical connectivity in vegetative state: preliminary investigation using PET. Neuroimage 1999 ; 9 : 377-382. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Levy DE, Sidtis JJ, Rottenberg DA, et al. Differences in cerebral blood flow and glucose utilization in vegetative versus locked-in patients. Ann Neurol 1987 ; 22 : 673-682. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Laureys S, Faymonville ME, Moonen G, et al. PET scanning and neuronal loss in acute vegetative state. Lancet 2000 ; 355 : 1825-1827. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  18. Laureys S, Lemaire C, Maquet P, et al. Cerebral metabolism during vegetative state, after recovery to consciousness. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1999 ; 67 : 121. [Google Scholar]
  19. Laureys S, Boly M, Moonen G, Maquet P. Coma In: Squire L, ed. Encyclopedia of neuroscience. Amsterdam, NY : Elsevier, 2000 : 1133-1142. [Google Scholar]
  20. Laureys S, Owen AM, Schiff ND. Brain function in coma, vegetative state, and related disorders. Lancet Neurol 2004 ; 3 : 537-546. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Laureys S, Faymonville ME, Luxen A, et al. Restoration of thalamocortical connectivity after recovery from persistent vegetative state. Lancet 2000 ; 355 : 1790-1791. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Boly M, Faymonville ME, Peigneux P, et al. Auditory processing in severely brain injured patients: differences between the minimally conscious state and the persistent vegetative state. Arch Neurol 2004 ; 61 : 233-238. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Laureys S, Perrin F, Faymonville ME, et al. Cerebral processing in the minimally conscious state. Neurology 2004 ; 63 : 916-918. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Boly M, Faymonville ME, Schnakers C, et al. Perception of pain in the minimally conscious state with PET activation: an observational study. Lancet Neurol 2008 ; 7 : 1013-1020. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Boly M, Coleman MR, Davis MH, et al. When thoughts become action: an fMRI paradigm to study volitional brain activity in non-communicative brain injured patients. Neuroimage 2007 ; 36 : 979-992. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Owen AM, Coleman MR, Boly M, et al. Detecting awareness in the vegetative state. Science 2006 ; 313 : 1402. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Monti M, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Coleman M, et al. Willful modulation of brain activity in disorders of consciousness. N Engl J Med 2010 ; 362 : 579-589. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Sorger B, Dahmen B, Reithler J, et al. Another kind of BOLD response: answering multiple-choice questions via online decoded single-trial brain signals. Prog Brain Res 2009 ; 177 : 275-292. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Mazoyer B, Zago L, Mellet E, et al. Cortical networks for working memory and executive functions sustain the conscious resting state in man. Brain Res Bull 2001 ; 54 : 287-298. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, et al. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2001 ; 98 : 676-682. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  31. Raichle ME. Cognitive neuroscience. Bold insights. Nature 2001 ; 412 : 128-130. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Boly M, Tshibanda L, Vanhaudenhuyse A, et al. Functional connectivity in the default network during resting state is preserved in a vegetative but not in a brain dead patient. Hum Brain Map 2009 ; 30 : 2393-2400. [Google Scholar]
  33. Vanhaudenhuyse A, Noirhomme Q, Tshibanda LJ, et al. Default network connectivity reflects the level of consciousness in non-communicative brain-damaged patients. Brain 2010 ; 133 : 161-171. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Fransson P, Marrelec G. The precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex plays a pivotal role in the default mode network: Evidence from a partial correlation network analysis. Neuroimage 2008 ; 42 : 1178-1184. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hagmann P, Cammoun L, Gigandet X, et al. Mapping the structural core of human cerebral cortex. PLoS Biol 2008 ; 6 : e159. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Current usage metrics show cumulative count of Article Views (full-text article views including HTML views, PDF and ePub downloads, according to the available data) and Abstracts Views on Vision4Press platform.

Data correspond to usage on the plateform after 2015. The current usage metrics is available 48-96 hours after online publication and is updated daily on week days.

Initial download of the metrics may take a while.