Free Access
Issue
Med Sci (Paris)
Volume 32, Number 12, Décembre 2016
Page(s) 1097 - 1102
Section M/S Revues
DOI https://doi.org/10.1051/medsci/20163212013
Published online 03 January 2017
  1. Butterworth B. The mathematical brain London : Nelson, 1999 : 480 p. [Google Scholar]
  2. Andres M, Di Luca S, Pesenti M. Finger-counting : the missing tool? Behav Brain Sci 2008 ; 31 : 642–643. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  3. Crollen V, Noël MP. The role of fingers in the development of counting and arithmetic skills. Acta Psychol 2015 ; 156 : 37–44. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  4. Fayol M, Seron X, Campbell JID. About numerical representations : insights from neuropsychological, experimental and developmental studies. Handbook of mathematical cognition New York : Psychology Press, 2005 : 3–22. [Google Scholar]
  5. Fuson KC, Richards J, Briars DJ, Brainerd CJ. The acquisition and elaboration of the number word sequence. Children’s logical and mathematical cognition New York : Springer-Verlag, 1982 : 33–92. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  6. Alibali MW, DiRusso AA. The function of gesture in learning to count : more than keeping track. Cogn Dev 1999 ; 14 : 37–56. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  7. Wiese H. Iconic and non-iconic stages in number development : the role of language. Trends Cogn Sci 2003 ; 7 : 385–390. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Wiese H. Numbers, language, and the human mind Cambridge, MA : Cambridge University Press, 2003 : 360 p. [Google Scholar]
  9. Noël MP. Finger gnosia : a predictor of numerical abilities in children? Child Neuropsychol 2005 ; 11 : 413–430. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Di Luca S, Granà A, Semenza C, et al. Finger-digit compatibility in Arabic numerical processing. Q J Exp Psychol 2006 ; 59 : 1648–1663. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Imbo I, Vandierendonck A, Fias W. Passive hand move-ments disrupt adults’ counting strategies. Front Psychol 2011 ; 2 : 201. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Michaux N, Masson N, Pesenti M, Andres M. Selective interference of finger movements on basic addition and subtraction problem solving. Exp Psychol 2013 ; 60 : 197–205. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  13. Pinel P, Piazza M, Le Bihan D, Dehaene S. Distributed and overlapping cerebral representations of number, size, and luminance during comparative judgments. Neuron 2004 ; 41 : 983–993. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Gerstmann J. Zur symptomatologie der hirnläsionen im übergangsgebiet der unteren parietal-und mittleren occipitalwindung. Nervenarzt 1930 ; 3 : 691–695. [Google Scholar]
  15. Rusconi E, Walsh V, Butterworth B. Dexterity with numbers : rTMS over left angular gyrus disrupts finger gnosis and number processing. Neuropsychologia 2005 ; 43 : 1609–1624. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Andres M, Seron X, Olivier E. Contribution of hand motor circuits to counting. J Cogn Neurosci 2007 ; 19 : 563–576. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Butterworth B. What counts. How every brain is hardwired for math. New York, NY : The Free Press, 1999 : 432 p. [Google Scholar]
  18. Butterworth B. The development of arithmetical abilities. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2005 ; 46 : 3–18. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Anderson ML. Neural reuse : a fundamental organizational principle of the brain. Behav Brain Sci 2010 ; 33 : 245–313. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Penner-Wilger M, Anderson ML. The relation between finger gnosis and mathematical ability : why redeployment of neural circuits best explains the finding. Front Psychol. 2013 ; 4 : 877. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Crollen V, Mahe R, Collignon O, Seron X. The role of vision in the development of finger-number interactions : finger-counting and finger-montring in blind children. J Exp Child Psychol 2011 ; 109 : 525–539. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Crollen V, Noël MP, Seron X, et al. Visual experience influences the interactions between fingers and numbers. Cognition 2014 ; 133 : 91–96. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Dehaene S, Bossini S, Giraux P. The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. J Exp Psychol Gen 1993 ; 122 : 371–396. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  24. Calabria M, Rossetti Y. Interference between number processing and line bisection : a methodology. Neuropsychologia 2005 ; 43 : 779–783. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Jewell G, McCourt ME. Pseudoneglect : a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks. Neuropsychologia 2000 ; 38 : 93–110. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Longo MR, Lourenco SF. Spatial attention and the mental number line : evidence for characteristic biases and compression. Neuropsychologia 2007 ; 45 : 1400–1407. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Longo MR, Lourenco SF. Bisecting the mental number line in near and far space. Brain Cogn 2010 ; 72 : 362–367. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Cattaneo Z, Fantino M, Silvanto J, et al. Tapping effects on numerical bisection. Exp Brain Res 2011 ; 208 : 21–28. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Charbonneau G, Véronneau M, Boudrias-Fournier C, et al. The ventriloquist in periphery : impact of eccentricity-related reliability on audio-visual localization. J Vis 2013 ; 13 : 20. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  30. Cooper RG, Sophian C. Early number development : discovering number space with addition and subtraction. Origins of cognitive skills Hillsdale, NJ : Erlbaum, 1984 : 157–192. [Google Scholar]
  31. Simon TJ. Reconceptualizing the origins of number knowledge : a non numerical account. Cogn Dev 1997 ; 12 : 349–372. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  32. Castronovo J, Seron X. Semantic numerical representation in blind subjects : the role of vision in the spatial format of the mental number line. Q J Exp Psychol 2007 ; 60 : 101–119. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Szücs D, Csépe V. The parietal distance effect appears in both the congenitally blind and matched sighted controls in an acoustic number comparison task. Neurosci Lett 2005 ; 384 : 11–16. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Cattaneo Z, Fantino M, Silvanto J, et al. Blind individuals show pseudoneglect in bisecting numerical intervals. Atten Percept Psychophys 2011 ; 73 : 1021–1028. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Rinaldi L, Vecchi T, Fantino M, et al. The effect of hand movements on numerical bisection judgments in early blind and sighted individuals. Cortex 2015 ; 71 : 76–84. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Crollen V, Dormal G, Seron X, et al. Embodied numbers : the role of vision in the development of number-space interactions. Cortex 2013 ; 49 : 276–283. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Herrera A, Macizo P, Semenza C. The role of working memory in the association between number magnitude and space. Acta Psychol 2008 ; 128 : 225–237. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  38. van Dijck JP, Gevers W, Fias W. Numbers are associated with different types of spatial information depending on the task. Cognition 2009 ; 113 : 248–253. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Bottini R, Crepaldi D, Casasanto D, et al. Space and time in the sighted and blind. Cognition 2015 ; 141 : 67–72. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Salillas E, El Yagoubi R, Semenza C. Sensory and cognitive processes of shifts of spatial attention induced by numbers : an ERP study. Cortex 2008 ; 44 : 406–413. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Current usage metrics show cumulative count of Article Views (full-text article views including HTML views, PDF and ePub downloads, according to the available data) and Abstracts Views on Vision4Press platform.

Data correspond to usage on the plateform after 2015. The current usage metrics is available 48-96 hours after online publication and is updated daily on week days.

Initial download of the metrics may take a while.